CANNABIS :
OUR POSITION FOR A CANADIAN PUBLIC POLICY
REPORT OF THE SENATE SPECIAL COMMITTEE ON ILLEGAL DRUGS
VOLUME I : PARTS I and II
Chapter
8
Driving
under the influence of cannabis[1]
Stan
Thompson was 18 when he and four other teenagers from Kanata were killed in a
horrific car accident near Perth that summer day. A youth was found responsible
for the fatal accident and served eight months of a 12-month sentence. Cannabis
and alcohol-impaired driving was the cause. … The year following Stan's death,
his father, Greg Thompson, went to local high schools to talk about the tragedy.
He spoke to students about what went wrong and how the tragedy could have been
prevented. … His message was that driving a vehicle and smoking marijuana does
and will affect driving abilities. He pleaded with the kids not to do it. …
Cannabis is not a benign substance. There is very little in the way of research
that allows anyone to determine levels of impairment related to cannabis and
driving ability, much less the levels of impairment related to cannabis and
alcohol and driving ability. We have seen in the Manitoba survey, over one-half
of the kids that are using cannabis do so in cars and during school hours. There
is no technical or scientific ability to test for cannabis impairment. We do not
have the technology, scientific data or the research. We do not have the proper
legislation. Studies done in British Columbia indicate that 12 per cent to 14
per cent of the drivers involved fatal motor vehicle accidents had cannabis in
their systems. The Government of Quebec and the insurance board in Quebec are
presently doing road surveys where people are voluntarily submitting to urine or
blood tests. The findings in these tests are that between 12 per cent and 14 per
cent of those drivers has cannabis in their system while driving. [2]
If
there is one issue, other than the effects of cannabis use on young people or
the effects of substance abuse, that is likely to be of concern to society and
governments, then it is certainly the issue of how it affects the ability to
drive a vehicle. We are already familiar with the effects of alcohol on driving,
and the many accidents involving injuries or deaths to young people. In spite of
the decreases in use noted in recent years, it is not difficult to admit that
one fatal accident caused by the use of a substance is already one accident too
many.
As it
happens, after alcohol, cannabis is the most widely used psychoactive substance,
particularly among young people in the 16-25 age group. Casual use occurs most
often in a festive setting, at weekend parties, often also accompanied by
alcohol. People in this age group are also the most likely to have a car
accident and are also susceptible to having an accident while impaired.
We
have seen that cannabis affects psychomotor skills for up to five hours after
use. The psychoactive effects of cannabis are also dependent on the amount used,
the concentration of THC and the morphology, experience and expectations of
users. But what are the specific effects of cannabis on the ability to drive
motor vehicles? What are the effects of alcohol and cannabis combined? And what
tools are available to detect the presence of a concentration of THC that is
likely to significantly affect the psychomotor skills involved in vehicle
operation?
Here
again, the witnesses heard by the Committee vary in their interpretation of the
study results. Thus, the Canadian Police Association told us:
Driving
while intoxicated by drugs impairs judgment and motor coordination. In one study
involving aircraft 10 licensed pilots were given one marijuana joint containing
19 milligrams of THC - a relatively small amount. Twenty-four hours after
smoking the joint, they were tested in a flight simulator. All 10 of the pilots
made errors in landing and one missed the runway completely. [3]
Two
weeks later, Dr. John Morgan of the City University of New York Medical School
said in reference to the same study:
A
California-based scientist named Jerome Yesavage wrote the study. It was done in
the early 1980s, I think, and it attracted enormous attention. … Doctor
Yesavage's study … was completely uncontrolled. … As you all have heard, it
is difficult to control for marijuana use. When Doctor Yesavage was funded by
the federal government to repeat the study with the simple controls that others
and I had suggested, they were unable to show any impact of marijuana use after
four hours in a similar group of people. Therefore, I believe that the truth is
that marijuana use will impact airplane and driving simulators and to some
degree driving performance for three hours to four hours after use; however
there is no sustained impact. Any impact is relatively minor. [4]
Making
reference to Robbe’s work, which we will be examining in greater detail in
this chapter, Professor Morgan added:
A
Dutch scientist who has for years worked on driving experiments found that
marijuana using drivers have a little difficulty staying right in the middle of
the road. That is most sensitive test. If you smoke marijuana, you tend to weave
a little bit more than completely sober people do. That is important, although
there have been no studies to show that that amount of weaving had a gross
impact on driving ability.
The
Dutch scientist included in his report that the amount of weaving was
approximately the same in individuals consuming very small amounts of alcohol,
very small doses of bensodiazopenes and very small doses of antihistamines. [5]
On the
same day, Professor Kalant of the University of Toronto responded as follows:
Dr. Morgan referred to some
experimental studies this morning. A number of studies, reviewed by Dr. Smiley
in the report of the World Health Organization Committee on Health Effects of
Cannabis, indicate a fair measure of agreement on what the predominant effects
on driving are. The lane control, as Dr. Morgan mentioned, is impaired. The
person does not steer as accurately. In addition, there was slower starting time
and slower braking time. There was decreased visual search. In other words, when
you drive, you must monitor for sources of danger to both sides and not just
ahead of you. There was decreased monitoring, decreased recognition of danger
signals. The effects were synergistic with those of alcohol. The one favourable
thing about cannabis compared with alcohol was that there was less
aggressiveness in the cannabis smokers than in the drinkers, so they were less
likely to pass dangerously or to speed. Nevertheless, driving ability was
impaired not just by weaker, poorer steering control, but also by less alertness
to unexpected things that might happen and pose a hazard.
I will not go into the
statistics of actual field studies of the involvement of cannabis in driving
accidents. However, I would like to say that a number of studies have shown that
there has been evidence of cannabis presence in the blood or the urine of people
who have been stopped for impaired driving who did not have alcohol present. [6]
As we
can see, and as was the case with respect to the effects and consequences on the
health of users, there are divergent opinions about the interpretation of
studies and their meaning in connection with the specific effects of marijuana
on driving.
This
chapter is divided into three sections. The first considers the ways of testing
for the presence of cannabinoids in the body. The second analyses studies on the
known prevalence of impaired driving, in both accident and non-accident
contexts. The third and last summarizes what is known about the effects of
cannabis on driving based on both laboratory and field studies. As in the other
chapters, the Committee will then draw its own conclusions.
Forms
of testing
There
are five known media for testing the presence of cannabinoids in the organism:
blood, urine, saliva, hair and perspiration.
Blood is
the most appropriate medium for detecting recent cannabis use because only a
blood analysis can distinguish between the active ingredients of cannabis and
metabolites that have no psychoactive effects. However, as we have already seen,
blood concentrations of D9THC peak
9 minutes after smoking; after 10 minutes only two-thirds of the concentration
remains, and it is down to 5 to 10% at the end of an hour; after two hours, it
becomes difficult to detect. Thus not all methods are appropriate for testing
because of the strong possibility of obtaining false negatives and false
positives. The most reliable method, gas chromatography using mass spectrometry
for detection, is extremely sensitive and can also estimate the time that has
elapsed between the most recent use and the taking of the blood sample.
We saw
in Chapter 7 that there was a dose-response relationship: 25 puffs affect
cognition more than do 10 puffs, and 10 have more of an effect than 4. But not
much data is available on the relationship between concentration and effects on
people, and the ability to answer the key road safety question, namely at what
concentration can one consider that faculties are impaired? In France, the D9THC
level that constitutes testing positive has been set at 1ng/ml[7]
for drivers involved in fatal accidents. Another author has come up with a
formula that establishes a relationship between D9THC,
11-OH D9THC
and D9THC-COOH
to determine a cannabis influence factor with a positive threshold of 10ng/ml.
An equal concentration of D9THC and
COOH suggest use approximately 30 minutes beforehand, and hence a very high
probability of psychoactive effects, whereas a higher concentration of COOH than
D9THC
suggests that use was more than 40 minutes beforehand. However, a concentration
of COOH in excess of 40 μg/l would indicate a chronic user, and hence it
becomes impossible to determine when the last use occurred. Other research has
established that a blood concentration of 10 to 15 ng/ml suggests recent use,
without however being able to give an exact figure.[8]
Urine
tests are also frequently employed and remain the most appropriate method for
rapidly determining whether subjects have been using. On the other hand, traces
of cannabis can remain in urine for weeks. Furthermore, the traces that remain
are of D9THC-COOH,
an inactive metabolite. Consequently, urinalyses are primarily useful for
epidemiological measurements of cannabis use, and cannot contribute to
information about impaired driving.
The
levels of concentration of D9THC-COOH
in urine are very high: for someone who smokes a joint a day, the level is
between 50 to 500 ng/ml and may reach several thousands ng/ml in heavy users;
the currently recommended threshold level for testing positive is 50ng/ml urine.
Saliva
is a very promising option for road safety because it is non intrusive and can
indicate recent use with some accuracy. The presence of D9THC in
saliva essentially results from the phenomenon of bucco-dental sequestration
during inhalation. Concentrations are very high in the few minutes following
absorption, varying between 50 and 1,000 ng/ml, but then decline very quickly in
the hours that follow, though they remain detectable for an average of four to
six hours. The European ROSITA project compared the reliability of samples taken
from urine, perspiration and saliva compared to that taken from blood. Saliva is
by far the most reliable, showing an exact correlation in 91% of cases. However,
the low level of concentration during the period when the psychoactive effects
are active means that sensitive analytical methods are essential. There is
unfortunately not yet a sufficiently accurate and reliable rapid detection tool
that can be used in driving situations. Hence the driving detection tools
correctly identified only 18 to 25% of cases and led to many false negatives.[9]
Perspiration
is generally considered poor for detection purposes, because of the persistence
of D9THC
in sweat, and the fact that it is also excreted into sweat in small quantities.
Hair
looks very promising because the significant amount of D9THC can
determine time since and level of use (low, moderate, high). However,
concentrations are only a few ng per mg of hair, which requires highly efficient
testing.
The
following table, taken from the INSERM report, summarizes the main
characteristics of the various biological testing media; where available, we
have added the threshold detection level adopted.
Main
Characteristics of Biological Testing Media |
|||||
|
Primary
cannabinoids |
Maximum
detection period |
Useful
for |
Methodologies
available |
Threshold
for positive test |
Urine Saliva Perspiration Hair Blood |
THC-COOH (inactive) THC
(active) THC THC THC 11-OH
THC THC-COOH |
Occasional
use: 2 to 7 days Regular use: 7 to 21 days 2
to 10 hours Highly
variable Infinite 2
to 10 hours |
Identifies
use Identifies
recent use Not
useful Identifying
& monitoring regular user Confirmation,
identification, dosage |
Yes,
many rapid tests No,
no rapid tests No,
no rapid tests Yes,
CPG-SM Yes,
CPG-SM |
50ng
of D9THC-COOH per ml not
determined not
useful not
determined 1ng/ml
(France) |
In all
instances, the handling and transportation of samples and the toxicological
dosages are essential to the quality of the analyses.
There is
still considerable uncertainty about thresholds that make it possible to affirm
that the presence of D9THC
would impair the driver. Furthermore, there is still no reliable rapid screening
test to identify recent use (urine tests cannot do this). Moreover, other drugs
besides alcohol, including many types of prescription medicines, may have an
impact on driving. That is why many authors, and a number of witnesses,
suggested to us that Canada adopt the Drug Evaluation and Classification Program
(DEC) and recognize police officers trained as Drug Recognition Experts. This
practice has now been adopted in most U.S. states (at least 34, as well as the
District of Columbia), British Columbia, Australia, Norway and Sweden.
The
typical scenario for driving under the influence of psychoactive substances
other than alcohol is as follows: a vehicle attracts the attention of a police
officer, who pulls the vehicle over and questions the driver; if there are
reasonable grounds to believe that the driver is intoxicated, a breathalyser
test is administered; however, when the test yields a result below the legal
limit, the police officer may still not be convinced that the driver is capable
of driving, but how is this to be proven? Before, more often than not, the
police officer had to release the driver. As we have just seen, there are no
equivalents to the breathalyser test for drugs and medicines, and, for cannabis
in particular, traces found in urine in no way establish that use was recent. It
was in this context that the police officers working for the Los Angeles Police
Department developed the Drug Recognition Expert System (DRE) in the early
1980s. Police officers are given specific training in the detection of people
driving under the influence of psychoactive substances and in the use of the
DEC.
The
system allows police officers who have reason to believe that drivers are
intoxicated to call on an officer specially trained in drug recognition, who can
then evaluate the driver on the basis of a set of systematic and rigorous
factors that are recognized as signs of the presence of drugs. The process
involves 12 steps:
·
Breath alcohol
test: This test will have been conducted by the police officer who stopped the
vehicle. The Drug Recognition Expert is only called in when the test is
negative.
·
Interview by
the arresting officer: The DRE asks the arresting officer a series of
conventional questions: in what condition did he or she find the suspect, what
he or she had observed, if he or she found drugs in the vehicle, suspect’s
statement, etc.
·
Preliminary
examination (the first of three pulse measurements): This involves determining
whether there are reasonable grounds to suspect the presence of drugs, and hence
eliminate the possibility that there is a medical condition. The DRE observes
the suspect’s overall condition, and questions the suspect about health,
examines the pupils and gaze, and takes the first of three pulse measurements.
If the DRE feels that there are no signs, the suspect is released. If the
condition is medical, a medical evaluation is requested. However, if drugs are
suspected, the examination continues
·
Examination of
the eyes: This consists of three tests: horizontal gaze, vertical gaze and
convergence. Apparently when under the influence of any drug, it is impossible
to have an involuntary jerky movement of the pupils on the vertical axis without
first provoking such movements on the horizontal axis. Thus if there are only
vertical jerky pupil movements, it is likely a medical condition (e.g. brain
damage). If there is horizontal jerkiness, there are likely drugs involved. To
determine horizontal movements, the DRE moves a pen or other object horizontally
in front of the suspect’s eyes. For vertical movement, the pen is moved from
top to bottom. Furthermore, as certain drugs prevent eyes from being able to
converge towards the bridge of the nose, the DRE performs a convergence test by
placing the pen or object on the person’s nose and asking the suspect to look
at it
·
Divided
attention psychophysical tests: The tests include balancing, walking, standing
on one leg and the finger-to-nose test
·
Vital signs
examination: This is the second of three pulse measurements, as well as a
measurement of blood pressure and body temperature
·
Dark room
examination: This involves examining the pupils under four different lighting
conditions: room lighting, darkness, indirect light and direct light
·
Examination of
muscle tone: arm movements
·
Examination for
injection sites
·
Questions about
suspect’s drug use and living habits
·
Opinion: On the
basis of all the evidence, the DRE forms an opinion based on a reasonable amount
of certainty
·
Toxicological
examination: The purpose of this examination is to corroborate the analysis by
the DRE officer. The decision concerning prosecution is made only when the
analyses are returned.
The
system was standardized in the early 1980s with the assistance of the U.S.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. It was first tested in a
laboratory study.[10]
In the study, four Drug Recognition Experts evaluated subjects who had received
either a placebo or a dose of drugs. Neither the subjects nor the officers knew
who had received the drugs. In 95% of cases, the officers correctly identified
the subjects who had not been given drugs. In 97% of cases, they correctly
identified the subjects who had been given drugs and in 98.7% of cases, they
were able to determine which subjects were under the influence of drugs.
A field
study was then conducted in 1985, once again with the assistance of the Highway
Traffic Safety Administration.[11]
In the study, blood samples of 173 drivers arrested for driving under the
influence of drugs were analyzed by an independent laboratory. The study showed
that the analyses carried out by the Drug Recognition Expert officers correctly
predicted the presence of drugs other than alcohol in 94% of cases. In 79% of
the cases, the analyses of the officers identifying the presence of a specific
drug turned out to be accurate.
The
most complete study was carried out in Arizona in 1994. In this study, the files
of over 500 persons arrested for driving under the influence of drugs were
analyzed, and toxicological analyses were conducted. The study showed that the
toxicological analyses corroborated the conclusions of the officers in 83.5% of
cases. Similar studies conducted in other states yielded comparable results:
81.3% in Texas, 84.5% in Minnesota, 88.2% in California, 88.2% in Hawaii and 88%
in Oregon.
With
respect specifically to cannabis, the expected signs listed in the system are
generally the following: no horizontal or vertical shaking, but no convergence
in gaze, dilated pupils, accelerated pulse and high blood pressure.
In
short, given the limits of detection in the field of the influence of cannabis
and the results of these studies, it
would appear that it would be highly desirable to adopt the DEC and train police
officers in drug recognition.
Epidemiological
data
According
to a number of the witnesses we heard, more than 40% of people whose driving
abilities are impaired would drive under the influence of cannabis. Others have
said that approximately 12% of accidents causing injury could be attributed to
the use of cannabis. What do the studies reveal?
Data on
the frequency of driving under the influence of cannabis (whether on its own or
together with other substances) are, for obvious reasons, difficult to obtain.
First, for drivers involved in an accident, a positive breathalyzer test means
most of the time that no other measurements are taken because a blood alcohol
level above the legal limit is enough to take legal action. Second, the methods
available to detect the presence of THC are intrusive (blood, urine), unlike the
breathalyzer, and hence pose specific legal and ethical problems. Other forms of
measurements, such as saliva samples, do not, for the time being, allow roadside
detection. Lastly, in studies of all drivers, the consent of drivers is required
to take a blood or urine sample, thus limiting the possibility of generalizing
results. Nevertheless, we will summarize the main points of a number of studies
conducted in recent years.
Studies
not involving accidents
Two
types of studies were conducted: surveys of all drivers selected at random from
the flow of traffic at various times of the day and week, and studies where it
was presumed that the people were driving under the influence during police
checks. The following table, drawn from the various data available from INSERM,
summarizes these studies.
Detection and prevalence of cannabis in Europe and Quebec where no
accidents are involved[12] |
||||
Reference
country |
Population |
Detection
method |
Sample |
Prevalence
(%) |
No presumption of driving under the influence of
psychoactive substances
|
||||
Germany, Kruger et al., 1995 Netherlands, Mathtijssen, 1998 Italy, Zancanner et al.,
1995 Canada Dussault et al., 2000 |
All drivers Night drivers on weekends Night drivers on weekends Highway drivers (representative survey) |
Screening: FPIA saliva Confirmation: CG/SM saliva Screening: combined saliva, perspiration and urine test Clinical screening, clinical and toxicological check (blood, urine) Urine Saliva Breathalyzer (alcohol) |
2 234 (of 3 027) 293 (of 402) 1 237 2 281 2 260 5 281 |
0.6 5 1.5 5 (in progress) > 0.8 : 0.8 |
With presumption of driving under the influence of
psychoactive substances |
||||
Norway, Skurtveit et al., 1996 Denmark,
Worm and Steentoft, 1996 United Kingdom, Scottland,
Seymour and Oliver, 1999 |
Drivers Drivers Drivers |
Screening: immunoassay blood; Confirmation: CG/SM blood Screening: RIA blood Confirmation: CG/SM blood Screening: immunoassay blood; Confirmation: CG/SM blood |
2 529 317 221 640 |
26 10 17 26 |
In all,
it was observed that the detection rates for the presence of cannabis varied
between 1% and 5% when there was no presumption of impaired driving. However,
the missing data, which likely resulted from refusals to supply a sample, made
it impossible to draw clear conclusions. The studies with presumption of driving
under the influence of drugs had clearly higher results: between 10 and 26%.
These results do not necessarily reveal a much higher prevalence of driving
under the influence of psychoactive substances, but rather a higher level of
vigilance by the police. Indeed, as we shall see immediately, the prevalence of
cannabis detection in fatal accidents is no higher in Norway (7.5%) than in
other countries.
Studies
where an accident was involved
It
is difficult to compare studies between countries because the detection methods,
even in an accident context, varies widely from country to country. We wish to
note once again that simply finding traces of cannabis in drivers involved in
accidents is not necessarily a sign that its use was the cause of the accident.
Nor does the absence of any screening result mean that no one was driving under
the influence of cannabis.
The
following table, adapted from INSERM results, refers to a number of recent
studies in Europe, America and Australia.
Prevalence of impaired driving(ID) when there
are accidents [13] |
||||
Country |
Population |
Detection method |
Sample |
Prevalence of cannabis (%) |
Belgium Meulemans et al., 1997 Spain Alvarez et al., 1997 France, Mura et al., 2001 France, Kintz et al., 2000 Italy, Ferrara, 1990 Norway, Christophersen,
1995 United Kingdom, Tunbridge,
2000 Australia, Longo, 2000 Canada, Cimburra, 1990 United States, Logan, 1996 |
Casualty accidents (2-wheeled and cars) Fatal accidents with suspected ID Casualty accidents (control group: patients) Casualty accidents Injuries Friday night checks Injuries, non-fatal accidents Fatal accidents (including 516 drivers) Injuries (non-fatal accidents) Killed Killed |
Screening: urine Confirmation: urine CG/SM and urine blood comparison Screening: immunoassay blood Confirmation: CG/SM blood No screening Confirmation: CG/SM blood Screening: urine Confirmation: CG/SM urine and blood, saliva and perspiration tests Screening: EMIT urine Screening: immunoassay blood Confirmation: CG/SM blood Screening: immunoassay urine Confirmation: CG/SM blood Screening: immunoassay blood Confirmation: CG/SM blood |
1 879 979 420 (381) 198 4 350 500 394 1 138 516 2 500 1 169 347 |
6 (urine) 3.6 (blood) 1.5 not reliable 11.2 (10.8) 13.6 (urine) 9.6 (blood) 5.5 7.5 12 10 11 11 11 |
Three of
these studies are particularly interesting. The Mura et al. study (2001) shows a
significant difference by driver age: among 18-20 year olds, the D9THC was
present in 18.6% of drivers, and in 50% of cases it was present alone (without
alcohol). An earlier study by Mura (1999) had shown that cannabis was
particularly common among young drivers: from 35% to 43% in the under 30 age
group, with an even higher prevalence (43%) for the under 20s, whereas past the
age of 35, the prevalence drops to 3%.[14]
The
study by Kintz et al. (2000) is interesting primarily because it clearly shows
that, after alcohol (13.6%) cannabis is the substance most frequently present
among drivers involved in accidents (9.6%). This study also shows that in the
whole sample, the incidence of cannabis as measured by taking a blood sample
(9.6%) is close to the level of driving under the influence of alcohol (10.6%).[15]
Then,
Longo’s study is of special interest because of the size and
representativeness of the sample and the fact that separate analyses were done
of D9THC and D9THC-COOH.
The study detected the presence of cannabinoids in 10.8% of drivers: 8% for D9THC-COOH
alone and 2.8% for D9THC-COOH
and D9THC
together, thereby showing a lower percentage of positive tests for D9THC than
the other studies. Furthermore, as in the other studies, subjects testing
positive to D9THC
were younger and more often men.
Closer
to home, Mercer and Jeffery examined the toxicological analyses for 227 drivers
killed in traffic accidents in British Columbia between October 1999 and
September 1991.[16]
Samples had been taken during autopsies within 24 hours of death, which
according to the authors, may indicate an under-estimation of the presence of
alcohol or drugs. Of the 227 people killed, 186 (43%) showed no signs of either
alcohol or drugs, 83 (37%) alcohol only, 23 (11%) alcohol and drugs, and 21
drugs only. As for cannabis, 29 of the people killed (13%; 26 men and 3 women)
tested positive to D9THC-COOH,
showing an average concentration of 15.9 ng/ml. In the +alcohol/+drugs group,
(23 subjects), 17 tested positive to THC metabolites and 8 were also
positive to D9THC
(13%). For the 0alcohol/+drugs group, (21 subjects), 8 (all men) were
positive to D9THC–COOH,
and 4 to D9THC.
Even though the authors concluded that D9THC
/D9THC-COOH
was present in 13% of cases, which is a percentage comparable to most of the
other studies, only 12 subjects killed tested positive to D9THC with
or without alcohol and only 4 without alcohol.
Lastly,
a more recent epidemiological study dealt with 1,158 cases of fatal accidents
(391) or of cases of driving under the influence of psychoactive substances when
the percentage of alcohol in the blood was below 0.1 (767) reported in Canadian
forensic laboratories on November 12, 1994.[17]
The most frequent substances identified were benzodiazepines (590 cases),
alcohol (580), cannabis (551), stimulants (224), opiates (176) and barbiturates
(131). For cannabis, we get the following table:
Presence of
cannabis in Canada (1994) |
|||
|
Total |
with alcohol |
without alcohol |
THC Ø
Impaired driving Ø
Death THC-COOH Ø
Impaired driving Ø
Death |
181 198 127 45 |
129 98 29 24 |
52 100 98 21 |
In all,
cases in which D9THC
without alcohol was present accounted for 13% of the total, which is close to
the figure found in the other studies.
Out of
all the studies, it was found that the presence of cannabis among drivers who
were injured or killed varies between 3.6% (confirmed by blood analysis) and 13%
(unconfirmed). Where there was confirmation of the presence of D9THC
compared to D9THC-COOH,
the presence of the active substance decreases by half. In addition, the risk of
testing positive is much higher for young men than other drivers. These
conclusions are largely shared by other authors.[18]
Epidemiological studies
on youth
In
recent years, epidemiological studies on youth in the school environment have
asked questions about the frequency of driving under the influence of
psychoactive substances, cannabis in particular. In Ontario, the 2002 OSDUS
study described in Chapter 6 shows that 19.3% of the students had driven their
car one hour or less after having taken cannabis at least once in the past
twelve months.[19]
More interesting is that this compares with 15% who said they had taken their
car less than an hour after one or two drinks. In Manitoba, the survey of youths
in school reveals that almost 20% see nothing wrong in driving after taking
cannabis.[20]
Finally,
the Cohen and Kaal study on long term consumers had shown that no less than 42%
of the respondents in Amsterdam and 74% in San Francisco had driven their car
under the influence of cannabis.[21]
Risk
assessment
Given
the difficulties of conducting reliable epidemiological studies on driving under
the influence of cannabis, a number of authors have analyzed the probability of
responsibility and the risk ratio involved in the use of cannabis. These studies
distinguish between drivers who are responsible for accidents and those who are
not. The former are the subjects and the latter the control group. Comparisons
are then made of their intoxication to various substances. Clearly, placing
drivers into the two categories of responsible/not responsible may depend on an
investigator’s perception of whether or not psychoactive substances are
present.
The
following table, which is reproduced from the Ramaekers et al. report (2002) for
the International Scientific Conference on Cannabis summarizes the results of
various studies.[22]
It should be pointed out that the probability of responsibility for drivers
showing traces of cannabis (D9THC
and/or D9THC–COOH,
whether measured in blood or urine) is compared to the responsibility of drivers
involved in an accident not testing positive to any substance (including
alcohol). The risk ratio for drivers not testing positive to any substances is
1.0 and is used as a point of comparison to determine the statistical
significance of observed change in the risk level of impaired drivers. When the
reference value is above the statistical confidence level of 95%, the obvious
conclusion is that the drug is 95% associated with an increased risk of
responsibility.
Level
of culpability relative to driving under the influence of cannabis |
||||
Authors |
Substance |
Odds ratio |
Confidence interval at 95% |
N of drivers culpable / not culpable |
Terhune & Fell (1982),
U.S. Williams et al. (1985),
U.S. Terhune et al. (1992), U.S. Drummer (1994), Australia Hunter et al. (1998),
Australia Lowenstein &
Koziol-McLain (2001), U.S. Drummer et al. (2001) &
Swann (2000), Australia |
Drug free cases Alcohol THC Alcohol/THC Drug free cases Alcohol THC or THC-COOH Alcohol/THC or THC-COOH Drug free cases Alcohol THC Alcohol/THC Drug free cases Alcohol THC-COOH Alcohol/THC-COOH Drug free cases Alcohol THC Ø
£ 1.0 ng/ml Ø
1,1 – 2,0 ng/ml Ø
> 2 ng/ml THC-COOH Ø
1 – 10 ng/ml Ø
11 – 20 ng/ml Ø
21 – 30 ng/ml Ø
> 30 ng/ml Alcohol/THC No substance Alcohol THC-COOH Alcohol/THC-COOH No substance Alcohol THC THC > 5 ng/ml THC-COOH Alcohol/THC |
1.0 5.4* 2.1 - 1.0 5.0 0.2 8.6* 1.0 7.4* 0.7 8.4* 1.0 5.5* 0.7 5.3* 1.0 6.8* 0.35 0.51 1.74 0.69 1.04 0.87 1.62 11.5* 1.0 3.2 1.1 3.5* 1.0 5.7* 3.0* 6.4* 0.8 19* |
2.8 – 10.5 0.7 – 6.6 2.1 – 12.2 0.2 – 1.5 3.1 – 26.9 5.1 – 10.7 0.2 – 1.8 2.1 – 72.1 3.2 – 9.6 0.4 – 1.5 1.9 – 20.3 4.3 – 11.1 0.3 – 2.1 0.2 – 1.4 0.6 – 5.7 0.5 – 2.2 0.4 – 2.1 0.6 – 4.8 0.6 – 4.8 4.6 – 36.7 1.1 – 9.4 0.5 – 2.4 1.2 – 11.4 4.1
– 8.2 1.2
– 7.6 1.3
– 115.7 0
– 1.3 2.6
– 136.1 |
94/179 45/16 9/8 - 55/23 120/10 10/9 123/6 541/258 587/38 11/8 35/2 392/140 261/17 29/14 59/9 944/821 173/22 2/5 7/12 12/6 19/24 18/15 12/12 13/7 66/6 114/126 17/6 17/17 16/5 1209/372 720/39 49/5 24/0 68/26 65/62 |
The
study findings show that cannabis alone does not increase the likelihood of
responsibility in an accident. However, most of the studies used a measurement
of THC-COOH, an inactive metabolite that can remain in urine for several days.
When the authors separated out THC alone, the risk ratio was slightly higher,
even though it did not reach the required level of significance. In addition, as
the concentration of THC increases, the more the ratio increases, once again
suggesting a dose-response relationship. Furthermore, the cannabis and alcohol
combination significantly increases risk. Without being able to draw any
definite conclusions, there are some signs that their effects are in synergy and
not merely additive.
Studies
on injured drivers (Terhune (1982) and Hunter (1998)) have ratios somewhat
higher than in the other studies on fatal accidents. According to Bates and
Blakely (1999), the apparent reduction in the risk of a fatal accident stems
from the fact that drivers under the influence of cannabis drive less
dangerously, for example by reducing their speed.[23]
To
conclude, we are rather in agreement with INSERM concerning these studies:
[translation] The findings definitely confirm the significant risk of alcohol, but
generally fail to demonstrate that there is an effect of cannabis alone on the
risk of being responsible for a fatal accident or an accident involving serious
injury. The methodological difficulties that make such a demonstration difficult
contribute considerably to the absence of statistically indisputable results.
Analyses of responsibility nevertheless suggest that the association between
alcohol and cannabis increases the risk of being responsible for an accident,
compared to drinking alone; however, this finding needs to be consolidated.
Lastly, the most recent data tend to show that there is a risk of becoming
responsible at heavy concentrations of D9THC.
This involves using cannabis immediately before driving, and perhaps applies
also to chronic users. [24]
Experimental
studies
Epidemiological
studies indicate a relatively high level of driving under the influence of
cannabis, between 5% to 12% of drivers, mostly among young men. At the same
time, neither these studies nor the responsibility/risk analyses reach clear
conclusions concerning the role of cannabis in dangerous driving. Hence the
interest in studies on how cannabis affects driving ability and driving itself.
Studies on the psychomotor and cognitive skills needed to drive vehicles have
measured factors such as: motor coordination, reaction time, attention, visual
attention and deductive reasoning. There are two types of studies on driving:
simulated studies and field studies, whether on a track, in the city or on a
highway. Most studies focus on single doses for recreational users. They use
control group protocols and cross-linked protocols, including placebos and
comparisons with alcohol. However, they are limited by the fact that they mainly
measure the acute effects of single doses, making it difficult to determine
whether more experienced users would react in the same way. The following
sections examine both types of study.
Non-driving
activities
In 1985,
Moskowitz published a remarkable synthesis of studies on the psychomotor and
cognitive effects of cannabis.[25]
In this synthesis, he examined motor coordination, reaction time, tracking and
sensory functions. The author observed the following:
·
motor
coordination, measured by hand stability, body balance and movement accuracy was
significantly affected. However, the application of these results to driving a
car is limited, except in driving situations that require considerable
coordination, such as emergency situations. The limits in terms of dose and
number of subjects tested (between 8 and 16) also need to be noted
·
reaction time
was not significantly changed: “There
are a sufficient number of experiments involving both simple and complex
reaction time situations to leave us relatively well assured that neither the
speed of initial detection nor the speed of responding are, per se, impaired by
marihuana. Rather, when marihuana produces a reaction time increase, there is
some dimension of the information processing task which the subject must execute
which bears the brunt of the experiment.”[26]
Attention rather than reaction time was affected by marijuana use
·
straight line:
this dimension was particularly sensitive to the effects of marijuana, and the
vast majority of studies showed a significant reduction in the ability to go in
a straight line or correct deviations from the line
·
the sensory
functions (hearing and visual) are often affected, but the studies did not yield
precise results concerning the distinction between simple tasks and complex
tasks.
Ramaekers et
al. (2002), reported a
meta-analysis on 87 controlled laboratory studies on the psychomotor effects of
cannabis conducted by Berghaus et al. (1998). These authors found that the
number of psychomotor functions linked to driving (following, reaction time,
perception, hand-eye coordination, body balance, signal detection and divided
and continuous attention) affected by THC reached a maximum during the first
hour after smoking, and one to two hours after oral ingestion. The maximum
figures were comparable to those obtained with an alcohol concentration
equivalent to >
0.05 g/dl. The number of functions affected reached zero after three to four
hours, and only higher doses continued to have an effect. The studies surveyed
also showed that THC concentration in the blood is highly correlated to
psychomotor effects: a concentration of between 14 ng/ml and 60 ng/ml affected
between 70% and 80% of tasks.[27]
The
following table summarizes these data:
|
Deterioration of performance on
psychomotor tests by dose, time and method of ingestion |
||||
THC
dose |
Time
(in hours)
< 1
1-2
2-3
3-4
4-5
|
||||
|
Tests (n)
% affected |
Tests (n)
% affected |
Tests (n)
% affected |
Tests (n)
% affected |
Tests (n)
% affected |
Smoked < 9mg 9 – 18 mg ³ 18 mg Total Oral < 9mg 9 – 18 mg ³ 18 mg Total |
271
61% 193
53% 64
64% 528
58% 3
33% 3 0% 3
0% 9
11% |
33
36% 48
38% 28 36% 109
37% 49 14% 41 39% 45 60% 135
37% |
10 30%
8 38% 10 40% 28
36% 37 8% 45 18% 15 33% 97 20% |
10 0%
6 0% 15
53% 31
26% 13 8% 17 18% 15 33% 45 20% |
11 0%
2 0%
3 67% 16 13%
-
-
-
- 11
45% 11 45% |
More
recently, after surveying the studies carried out in recent years, the reports
prepared by INSERM and the International Scientific Conference on Cannabis
reached largely similar conclusions: cannabis affects reaction time where choice
is involved, road tracking, shared attention and continuous attention, as well
as memory processes, but does not significantly affect simple reaction time or
visual or eye-movement functions.
While
driving
One of
the weaknesses of the laboratory studies is the difficulty of relating
psychomotor and cognitive tasks directly to driving. Several tests measured in
these studies are short and relatively simple and do not necessarily reflect
real situations. The advantage of simulated driving studies and field driving
studies is that it brings the conditions closer to reality.
Most
contemporary studies have similar characteristics: subjects have had a
driver’s licence for at least three years. They are often regular cannabis
users. The subjects receive either cannabis or a placebo in a double-blind
situation that is very strictly timed to control the level of THC transmitted.
In some instances, the experimenters also include comparisons with alcohol and
an alcohol placebo. However, it is impossible to control how much subjects
inhale and actually absorb. The cannabis prepared by the U.S. National Institute
of Drug Abuse (NIDA) varies between 1.75% THC for low doses, 2.67% for moderate
doses and 3.95% for strong doses. Converted into mg/kg
of weight, the doses correspond to 100, 200 and 300 mg/kg,
whereas the heavy dose usually preferred by regular users is generally 308 mg/kg.
The subjects are familiarized with the equipment used and the tasks to be
performed, and are accompanied by instructors on actual driving studies.
Measurements include the standard deviation of lateral position in relation to
the road, the control over longitudinal position (distance) in relation to the
vehicle ahead, decision-making in emergencies, style of driving and risk taking.
The
following table, adapted from INSERM data, summarizes a number of the more
recent studies.
Effects of
cannabis on car driving[28] |
||||
Reference / environment |
Subject / Dose / Protocol |
Tasks |
Measurements |
Results |
Simulator Liguori et al., 1998 Sexton et al., 2000 |
10 users Placebo Cigarette 1.77% THC smoked in 5 mn Cigarette 3.95% THC smoked in 5 mn Test: 2 mn after Duration: 1 hour 15 users Placebo Grass, low dose 1.77% THC Heavy dose: 2.67% THC 1 resin cigarette: 1.70% THC Blood and saliva sample 10 mm after start Test 30 mn Duration: 25 mn |
Avoid a barrier that suddenly appears by braking (55 to 60mph) Judgment: maintain speed of 30mph on marked road and select widest lane
at intersection Highway section with vehicle ahead passing Highway section with vehicle ahead braking 16.7 km of highway section Left and right turns Intersection with traffic lights, with 4 lane road |
Total braking time Lag time to take foot off accelerator and step on brake Average speed Number of cones knocked over Number of successful choices Average reaction time Average reaction time Maximum, minimum and average speed Standard deviation for perfect line Response time in going through amber Average waiting period at a point 10m from the stop line |
↑ Slightly significant at 1.77 THC, slightly more at 3.95 No difference No effect ↑ At low dose (high level of variability at heavy dose: ns) ↑ At low dose (ns) ↓ Average of 6mph at low and heavy dose ↑ Variation at heavy dose versus low dose or placebo ↓ At heavy dose ↓ At heavy dose (high level of variability: ns) |
Actual driving Robbe, 1998 study No. 1 Closed portion of highway (cannabis) Study No. 2 Normal traffic on highway (cannabis) Study No. 3 City driving (cannabis) Study No. 3 City driving (alcohol) Robbe, 1998 Highway driving (cannabis and alcohol) Lamers and Ramaekers, 2000 City driving (cannabis and alcohol) |
24 users Placebo 100, 200 and 300 Test: 40 mm and 1 hour 40 mm after 16 users same doses as study 1 Test: 45 mn after 16 users Placebo 100 Test: 30 mn after 16 users Placebo Alcohol level: 0.5 g/l 18 users THC: 100, 200 Alcohol: 0.4 g/l Preparation: Alcohol 0 + THC 0 Alcohol ) + THC 100 Alcohol 0 + THC 200 Alcohol 0.4 + THC 0 Alcohol 0.4 + THC 100 Alcohol 0.4 + THC 200 Alcohol plus cannabis 60 mn after Tests between 9:00 p.m. and 11:15 p.m. 16 users THC 100 Alcohol 0.5 g/l 4 preparations: Alcohol 0 + THC 0 Alcohol 0.5 + THC 0 Alcohol 0 + THC 100 Alcohol 0.5 + THC 100 Tests: 15 mn after Duration: 45 mm |
Constant speed at 90km/hr and tracking over 22km Tracking control (Ibid.) 64km, 50 mn Following cars over 50m at variable speed (between 80 and 100km/h) over
16 km, 15 mn City driving 17.5 km Dense, moderate or light traffic Ditto Tracking: speed at 100km and constant lateral position Following: follow a vehicle over 50 m with speed varying by ± 15km/hr
every 5mn Driving in traffic City driving 15 km Visual search monitoring |
Standard deviation of lateral position Average lateral position deviation Average speed and standard deviation Same measurements Average reaction time Average distances and standard deviations External observations Internal observations: skill, manoeuvres, turns… External observations Internal observations: skills, manoeuvres, turns… Standard deviation of lateral position Reaction time Average distances and standard deviations Frequency of appropriate eye movements Quality of driving |
↑ Instability at all 3 doses No effect No effect Same effects ↑ ns Distance increased by 8, 6 and 2 m for 100, 200 and 300 THC No significant change No effect No significant change 0.34 g/l alcohol level modifies control and manoeuvres ↑ Tracking variability; low alcohol alone, THC 100 alone;
Moderate: THC 200 Heavy: alcohol 0.4 and THC two doses ↑ Reaction time for 0.4 alcohol and THC 200 ↑ Variability in distance between cars in all cases No effect with alcohol alone or cannabis alone ↓ Performance if alcohol + cannabis No effect |
It is
interesting to recall that one of the first driving studies on the road was
conducted for the Le Dain Commission.[29]
In this study, on a closed track, 16 subjects were each given the 4 following
preparations: placebo, marijuana 21 and marijuana 88 μg/kg THC and a
dose of alcohol equivalent to BAC 0.07. The tests were conducted immediately
after use and three hours later. The subjects were to complete six circuits of
the track (1.8 km) with manoeuvres involving slowing down while going forward
and backwards, maintaining a trajectory and weaving through cones. The alcohol
and heavy dose of marijuana decreased driver performance in tests conducted
immediately after use. At the heavy cannabis dose, drivers drove more slowly. On
the second test, the differences were less clear.
When the
results of this study are compared to those conducted more recently using much
more sophisticated methods, it can be seen that the results are remarkably
similar.[30] Thus the following was
observed:
·
lateral
control: this is the variable that is most sensitive to the effects of THC, but
the effects are variable, depending on the dose and time; only heavy doses
significantly affected lateral control over the vehicle. In comparison, alcohol
has a greater effect on vehicle lateral control and speed (linked variables)
·
speed control:
in almost all cases, the use of cannabis significantly decreases speed
·
risk-taking: in
addition to decreasing speed, it is generally found that there is an increase in
distance between vehicles among marijuana users, and less of a tendency to pass
or attempt dangerous manoeuvres
·
decision time:
this variable is particularly important in actual driving situations. The
results do not appear to be very consistent. Smiley suggests that reaction time
is unaffected when the subjects are told that they need to respond rapidly,
whereas on the other hand, when the obstacles are completely unexpected, the
subjects who used cannabis do not perform as well
·
combined
effects of alcohol and cannabis: when the researchers checked the effects of the
two substances, the combined effects of cannabis and alcohol were systematically
greater than alcohol alone or, even more so, than cannabis alone.
Lastly,
with low doses, subjects had the impression that their driving was not as good
as observers felt it was, which was not necessarily the case with higher doses,
where the perceptions of both the drivers and the observers agreed.
Conclusions
The
Committee feels it is likely that cannabis makes users more cautious, partly
because they are aware of their deficiencies and they compensate by reducing
speed and taking fewer risks. However, because what we are dealing with is no
longer the consequences on the users themselves, but the possible consequences
of their behaviour on others, the Committee feels that it is important to opt
for the greatest possible caution with respect to the issue of driving under
the influence of cannabis. Given what we have seen in this chapter, we conclude
the following.
Conclusions
of Chapter 8 |
|
Epidemiological
data Data on effects on
driving Testing Further studies |
Ø
Between
5% and 12% of drivers may drive under the influence of cannabis; this
percentage increases to over 20% for young men under 25 years of age. Ø
This in
itself does not mean that drivers under the influence of cannabis
represent a traffic safety risk. Ø
A not
insignificant percentage of drivers test positive for cannabis and alcohol
together. Ø
Cannabis
alone, particularly in low doses, has little effect on the skills involved
in automobile driving. Ø
Cannabis,
particularly in the doses that match typical doses for regular users, has
a negative impact on decision time and trajectory. Ø
Cannabis
leads to a more cautious style of driving. Ø
The
effects of cannabis when combined with alcohol are more significant than
for alcohol alone. Ø
Blood
remains the best medium for detecting the presence of cannabinoids. Ø
Urine
cannot screen for recent use. Ø
Saliva is
promising, but rapid commercial tests are not yet reliable enough. Ø
The
visual recognition method used by police officers has yielded satisfactory
results. It is essential to
conduct studies in order to: Ø
Develop a
rapid testing tool. Ø
Learn
more about the driving habits of cannabis users. |
[1]
In addition to the specific studies we consulted, which will be
referred to appropriately, this chapter is largely based on the surveys
carried out by INSERM (2001) op. cit.,
Ramaekers et al., for the International Science Conference on Cannabis in
Pelc, I., op. cit.), and Smiley
(1999) in Kalant (ed.) op.cit.
[2]
R.G. Lesser, Chief Superintendent, Royal Canadian Mounted Police,
testifying before the Special Senate Committee on Illegal Drugs, October 29,
2001, Issue 8, page 17.
[3] Dale
Orban, Detective Sergeant, Regina Police Service, for the Canadian Police
Association, testimony given before the Special Senate Committee on Illegal
Drugs, May 28, 2001, Issue 3, page 47.
[4]
Dr John Morgan, Professor at the City University of New York Medical
School, testimony before the Special Senate Committee on Illegal Drugs, June 11,
2001, Issue 4, page 40-41.
[5]
Ibid.
[6]
Dr Harold Kalant, Professor Emeritus, University of Toronto,
testimony before the Special Senate Committee on Illegal Drugs, June 11,
2001, Issue 4, page 75.
[7]
In this chapter, ng means nanogram (i.e. one billion of one gram) and
μg means microgram (one million of one gram)
[8]
INSERM (2001), op. cit.,
pages 152-153.
[9]
Ramaekers, J.G. et al., 2002 “Performance impairment and risk of
motor vehicle crashes after cannabis use” in Pelc, I. (ed.) International Scientific Conference on Cannabis, Brussels, page 81.
[10]
Bigelow, G.E. (1985) Identifying
types of drug intoxication; laboratory evaluation of a subject procedure.
Cited in Sandler, D. (2000) “Expert and Opinion Testimony of Law
Enforcement Officers Regarding Identification of Drug Impaired Drivers.” University
of Hawaii Law Review 23 (1), 150-181.
[11]
Compton, P.R. (1986) Field
Evaluation of the Los Angeles Police Department Drugs Detection Procedure.
Cited in Sandler, op. cit., page
151.
[12]
Table reproduced from INSERM (2001), op.
cit., page 175.
[13]
Adapted from INSERM (2001) op.
cit., pages 171 and 174.
[14]
See INSERM, (2001), op. cit.,
page 172.
[15]
Ibid.
[16]
Mercer, W.G. and W.K. Jeffery (1995) “Alcohol, Drugs and Impairment
in Fatal Traffic Accidents in British Columbia” Accid. Anal. And Prev., 27 (3), pages 335-343.
[17]
Jeffery, W.K. et al. (1996) “The involvement of drugs in driving in
Canada: An update to 1994.” Can.
Soc. Forens. Sci. J., 29
(2), pages 93-98.
[18]
Including the INSERM report (2001), op.
cit.; Ramaekers, J.G. et al., (2002) “Performance impairment and risk
of motor vehicle crashes after cannabis use” in Pelc, I. (ed.) International
Scientific Conference on Cannabis, Brussels.
[19]
Adlaf, E.M. et A. Paglia (2001) Drug
Use among Ontario Students 1997-2001. Findings from the OSDUS.
Toronto: Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, page 134.
[20]
Patten, D., et coll., (2000) Substance
Use among High School Students in Manitoba. Winnipeg: Addictions
Foundation of Manitoba.
[21]
Cohen, P.D.A. et H.L. Kaal (2001) The
Irrelevance of Drug Policy. Patterns and careers of experienced cannabis use
in the populations of Amsterdam, San Francisco and Bremen. Amsterdam:
University of Amsterdam, CEDRO, page 68.
[22]
Ramaekers et al. (2002),
op.cit., page73.
[23]
Cited in INSERM (2001), op. cit.,
page 192.
[24]
INSERM (2001), op. cit.,
page 194.
[25]
Moskowitz, H., (1985) “Marihuana and Driving.” Accid. Anal. Prev., 17 (4), pages 323-345.
[26]
Ibid., page 330.
[27]
Ramaekers J.G. et al. (2002), op.
cit., page 77.
[28]
Table adapted from INSERM (2001) op.
cit., pages 183-184.
[29] See
Hansteen, R.W, et al. (1976)
“Effects of cannabis and alcohol on automobile driving and psychomotor
tracking.” Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 282, pages 240-256.
[30]
See notably the survey of studies and the discussion in Smiley, A.,
(1999) “Marijuana: On-Road and Driving Simulator Studies” in Kalant, H.
et al., (ed) The Health Effects of
Cannabis. Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation, pp. 173 passim.